User Controls

What do you think is beyond the universe?

  1. #81
    iam_asiam68 African Astronaut
    now I need to go
  2. #82
    Originally posted by iam_asiam68 Actually, you are tossing out the Theory of Randomness.

    There is nothing random about genetic resistance..lolol if something is resistant to a poison then it's going to live..and it follows it's offspring will most likely carry that resistance too...what's random about that?

    DNA proves randomness is false.

    Your usually nonsensical statement.

    It's like if Irish only reproduce with Irish people. And then Irish repreproduce with Africans. The genetics are different due to cross breeding even though both Irish and Africans are human Species.

    SOME genetics are the same SOME are different...

    another example. When The Europeans came to America the native Americans had hardly any resistance to Small POX...the Europeans did. The native Americans had low resistance because they'd never been faced with the disease before...low resistance = high casualties...which means those that DO survive have a natural resistance...which will be passed on to some of the their offspring and over generations the overall resistance grows...

    Obviously in humans that progress is slow..in rats and bacteria it's measurable and has been and is fact.

    You don't seem to understand the basic principles at work here.
  3. #83
    Oh and don't forget to cite the medical professional who used the term 'bat shit crazy' in reference to someone they've never examined.
  4. #84
    Common De-mominator African Astronaut
    Mental illness.
  5. #85
    Originally posted by Jiggaboo_Johnson There is no missing link, never was.


    "evolation"
  6. #86
    Speedy Parker Black Hole
    Originally posted by iam_asiam68 You aren't very bright. The Garden of Eden has not existed since the evacuation. They can look forever and never find it. Ironically though, the Rivers listed than ran near it still do exist to this very day.

    You a dark, not dim dark. The gaps in your information are too vast to grasp the video.
  7. #87
    Originally posted by Jiggaboo_Johnson Yes it does. You probably need to first understand what the word means:

    "In biology, evolution is the change in the characteristics of a species over several generations and relies on the process of natural selection"

    Rats becoming genetically resistant to poison generationally = natural selection and a change in the characteristics (ultimately) of the species…when all the others die off, the change has been made. Again that's something that can be measured and has been within a human lifetime.

    FACTOID.

    poisons are man made.

    there rats that evolved to resist it evolved out of unnatural selection.

    ie, man selection by any other name.
  8. #88
    Speedy Parker Black Hole
    Originally posted by vindicktive vinny poisons are man made.

    there rats that evolved to resist it evolved out of unnatural selection.

    ie, man selection by any other name.

    man is natural
  9. #89
    Common De-mominator African Astronaut
    EVOL-TRON
  10. #90
    gadzooks Dark Matter [keratinize my mild-tasting blossoming]
    Originally posted by Speedy Parker man is natural

    True, but you don't see other species' deliberately separating and combining other species' in order to actively influence the their phenotypic traits.

    It's not unreasonable to call what humans do when breeding animals for desired traits something different from what happens when they are left to their own device.
  11. #91
    mmQ Lisa Turtle
    Zoos are sad.
  12. #92
    Originally posted by Speedy Parker man was natural

    corrected.
  13. #93
    Common De-mominator African Astronaut
    Originally posted by Speedy Parker man is natural

    Retarded take. Everything is natural. The reason why we have the concept of unnatural/synthetic/man-made is because it is a product of the specific subcategory of natural phenomenon known as a human. Unnatural vs natural is to distinguish stuff creates by man vs not created by man. You could do the same to any other creature but it's just not useful to us, there's nothing wrong with it.
  14. #94
    Ghost Black Hole
    Originally posted by gadzooks True, but you don't see other species' deliberately separating and combining other species' in order to actively influence the their phenotypic traits.

    WHAT ABOUT MICRO ORGANISMS THEY CAN STEAL YOUR GENETICS
  15. #95
    iam_asiam68 African Astronaut
    I am not finished yet but was thirsty for water.

    I find it ridiculous having to provide SOMEONE LIKE YOU with proofs (like I would lie) when on DH YOU WERE CAUGHT MANY TIMES BEING AN OUTRIGHT LIAR, PRETENDING TO BE A DEAD MAN, THEN PRETENDING TO BE THE DEAD MAN'S FRIEND, THEN PRETENDING TO BE THE DEAD MAN'S CO-WORKER, AND ON AND ON YOU WERE SO MANY PEOPLE RELATED TO THE DEAD MAN THEY SHOULD HAVE JUST SHOT YOU!!
  16. #96
    Originally posted by vindicktive vinny poisons are man made.

    Yes, I need to go refill some Water Moccasin fangs this weekend, they are getting low
  17. #97
    -SpectraL coward [the spuriously bluish-lilac bushman]
    Originally posted by Speedy Parker Here is the missing link.

    https://www.bitchute.com/video/PXOghvzbDwde/

    Before the Great Flood, they had technology to equal, and even surpass, our own. They even made it into space.
  18. #98
    Originally posted by -SpectraL Before the Great Flood, they had technology to equal, and even surpass, our own. They even made it into space.

    Well yes... Hurricane Harvey was only a couple of years ago.
  19. #99
    Speedy Parker Black Hole
    Originally posted by gadzooks True, but you don't see other species' deliberately separating and combining other species' in order to actively influence the their phenotypic traits.

    It's not unreasonable to call what humans do when breeding animals for desired traits something different from what happens when they are left to their own device.

    Are you second guessing nature?
  20. iam_asiam68 African Astronaut
    Originally posted by Speedy Parker Are you second guessing nature?



    I have permission to quote this:

    This is from my buddy who together we did select temperance, size, color, head size, walking, running when he asked me to help him with breeding Tibetan Mastiffs. He is a well known zoologist/Scientist who now lives in Denver, Colorado. This is his 32 years of work on RattleSnakes, BullSnakes, Spitting Cobra, Viper, and few other Snake Species.


    Charles W. Radcliffe's research while affiliated with San Diego Zoo and other places

    Overview

    Publications (37)
    Publication Preview
    Use of the vomeronasal system during predatory episodes by bull snakes (Pituophis melanoleucus)
    Article
    Sep 2013Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society
    David ChiszarCharles W. RadcliffeKent Scudder
    Four bull snakes (Pituophis melanoleucus) exhibited a higher rate of tongue flicking after constricting mice than after seeing and/or smelling mice for an equivalent length of time. Accordingly, it was concluded that the act of constriction potentiates chemosensory investigation mediated by the vomeronasal system. A comparably high rate of tongue flicking was also observed after the snakes swallowed a mouse. Since constriction and swallowing both involve grasping prey with the mouth but differ greatly in body postures, it is sug–gested that oral contact with prey (which stimulates the vomeronasal chemoreceptors) is the causative factor in the activation of chemosensory investigation.
    Cite
    Request full-text
    Publication Preview
    Distance traveled by mice after envenomation by a rattlesnake (C. viridis)
    Article
    Aug 2013Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society
    Karen EstepThomas PooleCharles W. Radcliffe[...]David Chiszar
    Adult mice (Mus musculus, C3H) envenomated by adult prairie rattlesnakes (Crotalus viridis) traveled an average of 185.6 cm in an open field prior to becoming immobilized (78 sec). The range of distances traveled by the 20 envenomated mice in this study was 0-676.5 cm. These distances give an indication of the extent of the trailing task that confronts a prairie rattlesnake under natural conditions. Hence, laboratory studies of trailing behavior in rattle-snakes should use trails of at least 180-200 cm if results are to have ecological validity. Of course, data from additional strains of rodents envenomated by additional species of rattlesnakes should be accumulated in order to supplement the information provided here.
    Cite
    Request full-text
    Publication Preview
    Cover-seeking behavior and ecdysis in red-spitting cobras (Naja pallida)
    Article
    Aug 2013Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society
    David ChiszarHobart M. SmithCharles W. RadcliffeJohn L. Behler
    During a study of cover-seeking behavior in red-spitting cobras (Naja pallida, N = 11), all but one of the snakes shed. We conducted a post hoc analysis that examined changes in cover-seeking prior to and after shedding, with the result that significant increases in cover-seeking were seen as early as 8 days prior to ecdysis. This corresponds to the time at which the earliest morphological signs of impending ecdysis make their appearance but is well before the time when visual and lingual sensory disruptions should be maximal. Accordingly, we suggest that cover-seeking is not elicited by the sensory disruptions of ecdysis, but that it anticipates them.
    Cite
    Request full-text
    Publication Preview
    Bushmaster (Lachesis muta) predatory behavior at Dallas Zoo and San Diego Zoo
    Article
    Apr 2013Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society
    David ChiszarJames B. MurphyCharles W. RadcliffeHobart M. Smith
    Bushmasters (Lachesis muta) that were observed during feeding episodes occasionally released rodents after the predatory strike. For one such episode, we report the presence of a sustained, high rate of tongue-flicking that lasted 136 min. In this paper, we also present photographs showing skin folds that cover the snake’s pits and eyes when a rodent prey was being held following the predatory strike. We suggest that L. muta may occasionally exhibit strike-induced chemosensory searching and trail-following when large (but ingestible) prey are encountered.
    Cite
    Request full-text
    Publication Preview
    Immobilization of mice following envenomation by cobras (Naja mossambica pallida)
    Article
    Feb 2013Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society
    Charles W. RadcliffeThomas PooleFrederic Feiler[...]David Chiszar
    Mice (Mus musculus, average weight = 20.8 g), envenomated by red spitting cobras (Naja mossambica pallida), were immobilized significantly sooner if the cobra delivered multiple strikes (3) than if it delivered only a single strike. Many viperid snakes (e.g., rattlesnakes) strike only once per predatory episode, whereas elapids (e.g., cobras) typically strike two to three times. The present study indicates that the number of strikes delivered by cobras is correlated positively with the severity of envenomation and inversely with the probability of losing potential rodent prey. Poststrike behavior of red spitting cobras is discussed, and is compared with that of rattlesnakes.
    Cite
    Request full-text
    Publication Preview
    Prey recognition learning by red spitting cobras, Naja mossambica pallida
    Article
    Feb 2013Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society
    Kathryn StimacCharles W. RadcliffeDavid Chiszar
    Eleven ingestively inexperienced red spitting cobras (Naja mossambica pallida; 4 weeks old) failed to respond to food items (neonatal mice, Mus musculus, and live liserds, Sceloporus undulatus). Live prey were inside clean plastic boxes that were placed into snake cages for 5 min. Rate of tongue flicking and number of biting attacks were recorded. The cobras were then offered neonatal mice (but not liserds), and gradually, these prey were accepted. By the end of the 5th week, all snakes had eaten at least one mouse. From this time until the snakes were 10 months old, mice were offered once each week, and most snakes ate each week. Prey recognition tests were conducted again (at 10 months), and the snakes responded to mice but not to liserds (Anolis carolinenesis). It is concluded that increased response to mice between the 1-month and 10-month tests was not based on increased predatory motivation and/or acclimation to the laboratory, because these factors should also produce increased response to liserds. Accordingly, it seems probable that experience with mice resulted in the acquisition of stimulus control by mouse-derived cues over snake predatory behavior (i.e., prey recognition learning).
    Cite
    Request full-text
    Publication Preview
    Duration of strike-induced chemosensory searching in cottonmouths ( Agkistrodon piscivorus) and test of the hypothesis that striking prey creates a specific search image.
    Article
    Feb 2011Canadian Journal of Zoology
    David ChiszarCharles W. RadcliffeRoy Overstreet[...]Thomas Byers
    Cottonmouths (Agkistrodon piscivorus) emitted significantly more tongue flicks after striking rodent prey than after seeing, smelling, and (or) detecting thermal cues from rodent prey. This strike-induced chemosensory searching (SICS) persisted for about 70 min. Prey-derived molecules acquired during the strike would not be expected to remain available to the vomeronasal organs for more than 10 min. Hence, the duration of SICS suggests (i) that a central nervous system (CNS) representation of prey is formed as a consequence of the strike and (ii) that this representation or search image has memorylike properties and remains available to guide searching behavior for a longer time than would be expected on the basis of poststrike residuation of chemically induced afference in the vomeronasal system. In experiment II, cottonmouths struck either rodent or fish prey (which were removed immediately after the strike) and 10 min later the snakes were allowed to ingest either a fish or a mouse. When the prey offered for ingestion was the same type as the prey struck, snakes grasped their prey quickly, whereas, in all other conditions, only a few snakes responded quickly and others did so after much longer latencies. It is proposed that CNS representations of fish and mice have some nonoverlapping features and that a disposition to grasp the type of prey that was initially struck endures until these prey-specific features have degraded (presumably through the ordinary process of forgetting).
    Cite
    Request full-text
    Publication Preview
    Strike Induced Chemosensory Searching in Cobras: (Naja naja kaouthia, N. mossambica pallida)
    Article
    Apr 2010Ethology
    David ChiszarKathryn StimacThomas Poole[...]Hobart M. Smith
    Predatory behavior was studied in 11 juvenile red spitting cobras (Naja mossambica pallida) and in subadult monocled cobras (N. n. kaouthia). Both taxa usually struck and released mice (Mus musculus) that were greater than 15 g, whereas neonatal mice were usually held following the strike. Large mice lived for 300–600 s after envenomation and were able to wander away from the site of attack. Snakes located the carcasses through searching behaviors which seemed to utilize chemical and visual cues arising from prey. Both taxa exhibited larger increases in rate of tongue flicking after striking (and releasing) mice than after seeing, smelling, and/or detecting thermal cues arising from mice. Strike-induced chemosensory searching (SICS) in these elapids was similar to behaviors previously described in many viperid species which have specialized on rodent prey. Although adult cobras may abandon the strike-release-trail strategy in favor of holding rodent prey after the strike, the behavior of the present juveniles and subadults was consistent with speculation that SICS is analogous in the Viperidae and the Elapidae.
    Cite
    Request full-text
    Publication Preview
    Behavioural consequences of husbandry manipulations: indicators of arousal, quiescence and environmental awareness
    Article
    Dec 1994
    David ChiszarW. Thomas TomlinsonHobart M. Smith[...]Charles W. Radcliffe
    Research to be described in this chapter was initiated by Conant (1971) when he called attention to the fact that cage-cleaning had arousing effects on numerous species of vertebrates at Philadelphia Zoo. His 1971 article focused exclusively on amphibians and reptiles, but his earliest experience along these lines was with mammals, as related in the following personal communication (1990) reproduced here by permission.
    Cite
    Request full-text
    Publication Preview
    Zoo and Laboratory Experiments on the Behavior of Snakes: Assessments of Competence in Captive-Raised Animals
    Article
    Mar 1993Integrative and Comparative Biology
    DAVID CHISZARHOBART M. SMITHCHARLES W. RADCLIFFE
    SYNOPSIS. Specific experiments on rattlesnake predatory behavior are described. Specimens of taxa bred in zoos are shown to behave qualitatively like wild-caught congeners, suggesting that the captive-bred animals have the skill necessary to hunt in natural habitats. Frequently, wildcaught conspecifics are unavailable for comparison with captive-raised individuals. Although this comparison is desirable, we must develop research strategies that can proceed without it. The qualitative analytical approach advocated here does this by relying heavily upon the natural history literature and on research with congeneric organisms to provide expectations (predictions) about the performance of captive-raised individuals. Advantages and disadvantages of this approach are discussed. We provide illustrations of its application to several predatory and antipredatory phenomena, and we list a variety of additional potential applications.
    Cite
    Request full-text
    Publication Preview
    Caudal Luring in the Southern Death Adder, Acanthophis antarcticus
    Article
    Aug 1990Journal of Herpetology
    David ChiszarDonal BoyerRobert Lee[...]Charles W. Radcliffe
    In six experiments on captive death adders (Acanthophis antarcticus), caudal movements increased in frequency when prey were nearby, and liserds (Hydrosaurus pustolosus) were attracted to the moving tail. We conclude that caudal movements of A. antarcticus serve a luring function, although they may have other functions as well. Two types of predatory caudal movements occur in death adders, those that are stimulated by prey and those that are occasionally made by snakes when no prey have been detected. The latter are conceptualized as "probes" designed to attract prey that might be out of the snake's view but still able to see the lure.
    Cite
    Request full-text


    Field observations on feeding behavior in an Aruba Island Rattlesnake (Crotalus durissus unicolor): strike-induced chemosensory searching and trail following
    Article
    Full-text available
    Jun 1990Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society
    Matt GoodeCharles W. RadcliffeKaren EstepR. Andrew Odum
    Observed a free-ranging Aruba Island rattlesnake after striking rodent prey and after no-strike presentations. Strike-induced chemosensory searching and trail following were seen after strikes. When a chemical trail was not present following a strike, the S searched extensively near its refuge but never emerged from it. Results suggest that the S was committed to cover, being willing to leave it only when a payoff was reasonably assured by the presence of a prey trail. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
    Download full-text
    Cite
    Publication Preview
    Source
    Chemical cues used by prairie rattlesnakes (Crotalus viridis) to follow trails of rodent prey
    Article
    Feb 1990Journal of Chemical Ecology
    David ChiszarTed MelcerRobert Lee[...]David Duvall
    Each of 10 prairie rattlesnakes (Crotalus viridis) was exposed to three types of trails after striking rodent prey (Mus musculus). One trail was made with mouse urine, another was made with tap water, and the third consisted of materials from mouse integument. The snakes exhibited trailing behavior only when integumentary trails were available. It was concluded that prairie rattlesnakes do not utilize urinary cues; instead they attend to materials associated with rodent skin and fur.
    Cite
    Request full-text
    Publication Preview
    The Predatory Strike of the Jumping Viper (Porthidium nummifer)
    Article
    Dec 1989Copeia
    David ChiszarCharles W. Radcliffe
    Cite
    Request full-text
    Publication Preview
    Cover‐seeking behavior in red spitting cobras (Naja mossambica pallida): Effects of tactile cues and darkness
    Article
    Dec 1986Zoo Biology
    David ChiszarCharles W. RadcliffeThomas BoyerJohn L. Behler
    Eleven red spitting cobras, Naja mossambica pallida, used clear Plexiglas hiding boxes as frequently as they used dark ones in a successive discrimination paradigm (experiment 1), which indicated that thigmotaxic cues can satisfy the cover-seeking needs of the snakes. In simultaneous discrimination tests, however, dark places were always preferred by the snakes (experiments II, III). Therefore, although thigmotaxic cues are sufficient, these plus darkness constitute a more favorable alternative for N m pallida. The husbandry advantages associated with clear hiding boxes, together with their sufficiency for the snakes, argue strongly for their use in many captive environments.
    Cite
    Request full-text
    Publication Preview
    Trailing behavior in banded rock rattlesnakes (Crotalus lepidus klauberi) and prairie rattlesnakes (C. viridis viridis).
    Article
    Nov 1986Journal of Comparative Psychology
    David ChiszarCharles RadcliffeFrederic Feiler
    Two rattlesnake taxa were compared in a task requiring 12 banded rock rattlesnakes to follow rodent trails. All Ss were wild-caught adults and had been in captivity feeding exclusively on rodents for 2 yrs prior to the present study. Banded rock rattlesnakes are primarily liserd eaters in nature but readily accept rodent prey in captivity. Prairie rattlesnakes are rodent specialists. Snakes of both taxa strike from ambush and typically hold liserds following envenomation, whereas rodents are released and allowed to wander freely while venom takes effect. Rodent carcasses are then located through chemoreception. Results show that the taxa exhibited some similar responses in the rodent trailing task, but C. l. klauberi had scores significantly inferior to those of C. v. viridis on several dependent variables (percent of tongue flicks and time on trail). It is suggested that greater dependence of C. v. viridis on rodents has brought about more effective trailing behavior than is seen in C. l. klauberi. Although it is possible that genetic differences are responsible for these behavioral differences, an alternative argument on the basis of differential early ontogeny cannot be eliminated. (23 ref)
    Cite
    Request full-text
    Publication Preview
    Trailing behavior in banded rock rattlesnakes (Crotalus lepidus klauberi) and prairie rattlesnakes (C. viridis viridis)
    Article
    Nov 1986Journal of Comparative Psychology
    David ChiszarCharles RadcliffeFrederic Feiler
    Two rattlesnake taxa were compared in a task requiring 12 banded rock rattlesnakes to follow rodent trails. All Ss were wild-caught adults and had been in captivity feeding exclusively on rodents for 2 yrs prior to the present study. Banded rock rattlesnakes are primarily liserd eaters in nature but readily accept rodent prey in captivity. Prairie rattlesnakes are rodent specialists. Snakes of both taxa strike from ambush and typically hold liserds following envenomation, whereas rodents are released and allowed to wander freely while venom takes effect. Rodent carcasses are then located through chemoreception. Results show that the taxa exhibited some similar responses in the rodent trailing task, but C. l. klauberi had scores significantly inferior to those of C. v. viridis on several dependent variables (percent of tongue flicks and time on trail). It is suggested that greater dependence of C. v. viridis on rodents has brought about more effective trailing behavior than is seen in C. l. klauberi. Although it is possible that genetic differences are responsible for these behavioral differences, an alternative argument on the basis of differential early ontogeny cannot be eliminated. (23 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
    Cite
    Request full-text
Jump to Top